When detectives in Dallas solved a 20-year cold murder case in early 2020, the celebrations rippled across the Atlantic. Needing verifiable evidence connecting the suspect to multiple murders over many years, the detectives drew a comparatively new UK-developed interview technique into their approach to obtaining information from the suspect.

Called the Timeline Technique, this interview format moves away from the conventional question-and-answer approach and instead requires witnesses to complete a detailed timeline of events. The process reduces reliance by investigators on 鈥榮tory鈥 narratives that gloss over details and place a heavy burden on interviewees鈥 memories and on the choice or nature of the questions asked.

In empirical tests, the Timeline Technique has been found to elicit a more complete and accurate chronicle of events. The Timeline Technique is part of an innovative set of interviewing and information elicitation tools developed in the UK and informed by psychological science. Another example is the 鈥榮elfadministered interview鈥 (SAI漏), developed by Lorraine Hope, a Professor of Applied Cognitive Psychology at the 黑料入口, with Fiona Gabbert (Goldsmiths, University of London) and Ron Fisher (Florida International University).

The technique is essentially a set of structured questions that witnesses complete by themselves. It was designed to help police capture the immediate memories of eyewitnesses to incidents in public spaces, such as terrorist attacks or other mass incidents.

鈥淚t prompts for all the details,鈥 explains Professor Hope. 鈥淭hen, it probes for information about the people who are involved, who else was present, were vehicles involved, what were the conditions under which you viewed, and so on.鈥

Professor Hope鈥檚 psychology research has come to have a direct impact, both in the UK and internationally, on how law enforcement now gathers evidence. It is an achievement recognised by the International Investigative Interviewing Research Group when it awarded her the 2019 Award for Academic Excellence.

Supporting witnesses

Professor Hope鈥檚 work addresses challenges relating to 鈥渕emory at the sharp end鈥, when witnesses 鈥 including victims, emergency services personnel and passers-by 鈥 have critical information needed by law enforcement.

鈥淢emory is not like a video recorder,鈥 she points out. 鈥淧hysical and psychological demands take their toll on a person鈥檚 cognitive resources 鈥 as well as factors such as having to make decisions and responses quickly. This means that crucial information may not always be optimally encoded. At other times the information is encoded, but witnesses need help to retrieve it from memory.鈥

It prompts for all the details. Then, it probes for information about the people who are involved, who else was present, were vehicles involved, what were the conditions under which you viewed, and so on.

Professor Lorraine Hope, 黑料入口

Memory in dynamic encounters

In other research aimed at assessing the effect of psychological stress on memory, Professor Hope monitored heart rates in pairs of police officers 鈥 one responding to an increasingly agitated armed offender threatening hostages and officers, and the other officer observing the simulation.

Afterwards, both officers had to give detailed accounts of what had happened. The active responder had a significantly higher heart rate and recalled significantly less correct information about the incident compared to the observer.

Almost 20 per cent of participants in this study recalled the offender pointing a gun at them 鈥 something that never happened. This illustrates an interesting and naturally occurring memory error.

Helping when resources are limited

The SAI漏 provides police with additional options for obtaining reliable information from multiple witnesses at a crime scene. In one of Professor Hope鈥檚 case studies, Greater Manchester Police helped test the SAI漏 after a chaotic road accident in which a speeding motorbike rider had crashed into a car, killing himself and landing his two pillion passengers in the path of an oncoming bus.

The first police officers on the scene identified eight key witnesses, to whom they spoke immediately. They took the names of another eight people who, under normal circumstances, would have been interviewed later. On this occasion, these witnesses were given SAI漏 forms. The feedback from police was that this saved them considerable time and helped them to identify three more eyewitnesses with useful information.

鈥淥ver time, your memory decays,鈥 Professor Hope says. 鈥淪o witnesses will be losing information from memory, such as valuable, specific details about what they saw. There鈥檚 also time for the memory to become contaminated, perhaps by new information about the incident appearing on the news, or false information being spread on social media.鈥

The SAI漏 now forms part of the College of Policing鈥檚 recommendations for obtaining initial accounts from witnesses in England and Wales. It has been translated into numerous languages and adopted either for use or in trials in Europe and internationally.

The SAI漏 has also been adapted for use in digital and online formats, while versions for other investigative contexts have also been developed including, most recently, a version to assist with missing persons investigations.

鈥淚n psychology, we ought to be able to deliver better tools and techniques for police to provide the best possible accounts of what happened, informed by objective science. There is no judgement in this 鈥 it鈥檚 all about the evidence,鈥 says Professor Hope.